logo

54 pages 1 hour read

Lauren Slater

Opening Skinner’s Box: Great Psychological Experiments of the Twentieth Century

Lauren SlaterNonfiction | Book | Adult | Published in 2004

A modern alternative to SparkNotes and CliffsNotes, SuperSummary offers high-quality Study Guides with detailed chapter summaries and analysis of major themes, characters, and more.

Chapter 8-ConclusionChapter Summaries & Analyses

Chapter 8 Summary: “Lost in the Mall”

Chapter 8, entitled “Lost in the Mall: The False Memory Experiment,” explores the research of psychologist Elizabeth Loftus, a University of Washington professor who has researched memory, and specifically the way memories can be distorted, since the 1970s and 1980s. Loftus has testified in court on numerous occasions, brought in by defense attorneys to discredit the memories of witnesses that detract from their case. Loftus has participated in the high profile trials of the Hillside Strangler, the Menendez brothers, Oliver North, and Ted Bundy. Years of research has showed how memories are “contaminated by the subtlest suggestion” (182). In the 1990s, when “recovered memory therapy” was coming to the fore, Loftus bucked the cultural trend and argued that repression is not always a believable phenomenon.

Around the same time, Loftus participated in a widely publicized trial, in which a daughter had accused her father of murdering her best friend nearly two decades prior, which she discovered via the recovery of a repressed memory. The father’s defense attorneys employed Loftus to discredit the daughter’s memory:

In one of the most publicized recovered memory cases of the decade, Loftus stood before the court and told of a mind that blends facts with fiction as a part of its normal course; she told how her subjects in the lab made red signs yellow, put barns in places where they never were, recalled black beards on bald chins (184).

Regardless, the father was convicted and sent to prison. It was at that moment that Loftus decided to make rescuing the falsely accused part of her life’s mission. Loftus set out to prove that memory could be entirely falsified, not just distorted. She devised an experiment called “Lost in the Mall,” which Slater describes as a “Don DeLillo-type trick that captures our national as well as individual absurdities” (186).

In the “Lost in the Mall” experiment, Loftus recruited college students to attempt to implant false memories in their siblings’ memories over the Thanksgiving holiday break. Loftus equipped each student with a booklet that contained four anecdotal accounts: Three of the accounts were true memories from the target’s past, one was a completely made-up story about the target being lost in a shopping mall as a small child. To test if the target would accept the false memory, Loftus had them write one-paragraph elaborations on each of the four anecdotes, embellishing them as best they could. Nearly 25% of the targets accepted the false memory of being lost in a mall.

Loftus’s findings have implications for victims. Judith Herman, the founder of Victims of Violence, says that Loftus’s research proves “the opposite” of what Loftus intends, since it seems that 75% of the participants did not accept the false memory. Psychiatrist Bessel van der Kolk specializes in trauma, and he is a major detractor of Loftus, given that Loftus’s research is often used to discredit abuse narratives:

According to Loftus, there is absolutely no substantial evidence that repression as a psychological or neural mechanism exists. Loftus instead posits that the rising of repressed memories is really a concatenation of fantasy, fear, innuendo, and news, with wisps of truth woven in (190).

Slater senses that Loftus’s tireless work ethic may be “driven by disassociated demons” (194), and so Loftus looks to her past to see if there is any trauma there. She discovers that Loftus grew up with an emotionless, mathematician father and a mother who suffered from deep depression. When Loftus was 14, her mother was found in the family swimming pool, drowned. It is unclear if her mother’s death was a suicide or an accident, but Loftus does not linger on this fact, returning to her work.

Loftus shares a letter that she composed to her deceased mother, in which (among other things) she wonders why she is such a work-a-holic. The chapter concludes on an ambiguous note: “In the end, then Loftus does not give me an answer about what she has, rather what she has not” (203). Slater surmises that, though memory can be falsified, it is nonetheless based on real pain:

In the end, there is this flash of insight and one woman’s plain pain. Maybe that’s all any of us have, just plain pain. No solid memories, but real regrets, regrets as substantial as stones—we can count on those. We can, like Loftus, pile those stones on top of the other, standing skyward, stretching out toward something (203).

Slater sees Loftus as having used her pain (the grief over losing her mother) as one of the primary agents that inspire her work.

Chapter 9 Summary: “Memory Inc.”

Chapter 9, entitled “Memory, Inc.: Eric Kandel’s Sea Slug Experiment,” covers the work of Eric Kandel. Kandel built upon Elizabeth Loftus’s work on human memory by trying to locate the biological basis of memory. He “set out on a journey to discover the actual workings of memory, its intricate cellular mechanisms” (204). Kandel was able to do this largely through experimentation and research on sea slugs. Chapter 9 is divided into three parts: “Part One” details the experience of a patient identified by the initials “H.M.,” who in 1953 underwent brain surgery for epilepsy and inadvertently led to a major scientific discovery about the hippocampus’s effect on memory. “Part Two” explains how scientist Brenda Milner’s research on H.M. furthered the study of memory. “Part Three” describes Eric Kandel’s work on memory, specifically his discovery of CREB, a biological molecule that can be considered, in many ways, as the basis of long-term memory.

“Part One” Summary

In 1953, a young man in Hartford, Connecticut, suffered from severe epilepsy. His fits were so frequent that his parents sought out the assistance of Dr. Scoville at Hartford Hospital, who offered the family an experimental cure: a lobotomy: “[Dr. Scoville] suspected Henry’s seizures might be kindling deep in the wetlands of the temporal lobes […] Scoville offered to excise Henry’s hippocampus” (205). After the surgery was performed, Henry’s epileptic seizures lessened, but he was unable to retain any memories: “A few weeks after Henry’s surgery, when his mental confusion did not clear up, Dr. Scoville realized he had inadvertently amputated the mill of memory, as well as the seizure’s starting point” (207). He inadvertently discovered that the hippocampus was hugely important to memory formation. 

“Part Two” Summary

Brenda Milner was a scientific researcher who studied Henry—who goes by the initials “H.M.”—after his lobotomy. Milner had been studying memory with a renowned physician Wilder Penfield when she first heard of H.M.’s case: “[Milner] had seen memory loss before, but H.M. offered her the chance to study the purest form of amnesia known to humankind” (208). Milner observed that while H.M. was unable to retain a conversation he had five minutes prior, he was able to walk, talk, and perform basic life functions—meaning, the long-term memory of how to perform these actions had been retained: “H.M. did not know, upon getting up in the morning, that he was supposed to brush his teeth, but once a toothbrush was placed in his hand, his hand took over” (208).

Milner deduced that the hippocampus “is clearly essential for memory of explicit, autobiographical detail—one might call it the core of consciousness itself—but there is another memory system located in a whole other place in the brain,” which Milner dubbed the “procedural memory” or “unconscious memory” (209). Milner’s discovery was critical in proving that memory functions on two levels, in separate systems of our brain.

“Part Three” Summary

Eric Kandel was a Harvard-educated scientific researcher who studied the cellular, biological basis of memory with the National Institute of Health. In an attempt to “elucidate the biology of memory,” Kandel worked extensively with sea slugs. He chose sea slug, specifically the breed aplysia, because they were easier to work with than the human hippocampus but contain the same kind of memory-functions that Kandel wished to study:

Aplysia has only twenty thousand neurons, many of them visible to the eye. Here would be an animal at once simpler to study, but still relevant to human beings because, as Kandel says, our nervous systems are the same, straight down to the food chain (211).

In his experiment, Kandel trained sea slugs to perform basic tasks and then studied the sea slug’s neurons as they learned a new task. Kandel learned that the links between neurons got stronger every time the slugs performed the same task, therefore engraining the behavior in their brains. In doing so, Kandel became “one of the first to actually provide a molecular model of primitive memory” (213).

Kandel’s next phase of research studied how short-term memory converted into long-term memory—i.e., what actually happens in the brain as behaviors are engrained into one’s long-term memory. Slater frames Kandel’s research as it relates to H.M.: “The fact that H.M. was able to remember the face of his mother even without a hippocampus suggests that the hippocampus is the binding site where memories go, to be wrapped in the cortex” (213). Kandel eventually discovered that a molecule called CREB is largely responsible for this conversion. In plain terms, the molecule CREB “dwells in the nucleus of a brain cell, and its purpose is to switch on the genes needed to produce the proteins that groove permanent connections between the cells” (214). Later, Kandel developed the Memory Pharmaceuticals company to create products to stop or reverse “the haze of age-related memory loss” (217).

As she ruminates on the ethical implications of memory-enhancing drugs, Slater considers the benefits of forgetting and the “dangers associated with too much memory” (218). Slater discusses an autistic patient who was not able to function in society but possessed an incredibly capable memory: “Show him The Odyssey and he could recite the thousand-page tome back to you after six minutes of staring, but he had no idea what it meant” (219). Slater also considers how it would benefit war veterans if we were able to produce drugs that could aid in forgetting. Slater concludes saying that, despite the ethical concerns, Kandel’s memory-enhancing drugs could prove beneficial to society overall: “Let us take Kandel’s medicine, if it comes to us, and return to people who have lost them their lives, pulling them out of the gap of forgetting that is wait for us all, if we live long enough” (222).

Chapter 10 Summary: “Chipped”

Chapter 10, titled “Chipped: This Century’s Most Radical Mind Cures,” explores the history of the lobotomy and psychosurgery in general. The chapter is divided into two parts: “Part One” describes the origin of the surgery in 1949 by António Egas Moniz in Portugal; “Part Two” examines a modern lobotomy case.

“Part One” Summary

The founder of the lobotomy, António Egas Moniz, was born in 1874 in a small coastal village in Portugal. He went to college in Lisbon, where he studied neurology. One of his main goals was to discover a way to view the brain: “Back in Portugal, [Moniz] wondered how one might visualize the brain. This all-important organ lay out of reach, encased in a cage of bone. If one could see the brain, one might be able to see the illnesses affecting it” (225). Moniz then developed a special dye that could be injected into a vein in the neck and, under X-ray, would illuminate the structures of the brain. This diagnostic tool was essential in Moniz’s next phase of research on the brain, one that would lead him to discover the lobotomy procedure.

Moniz first presented his idea that a lobotomy could heal psychological pain to a group of neurologists in 1935. Moniz was in London for a neurology conference, and other neurologists received Moniz’s concept of relieving anxiety and mental disorders by surgical means with disbelief: “History has it that everyone was startled, if not shocked by Moniz’s suggestion, the man maybe blinking and swiveling to see just who had spoken” (227). When Moniz returned to Portugal, he began practicing on cadavers. The first patient to undergo the procedure is known only as “Mrs. M.,” a 63-year-old woman who was severely depressed, anxious, and held the paranoid delusion that the police were attempting to poison her. Rather than using a knife to excise a portion of the brain, the first lobotomy was conducted by drilling “point-sized holes” on either side of Mrs. M’s skull, and then inserting an alcohol-filled syringe into her brain. The procedure was reportedly a success because weeks later Mrs. M began behaving normally, and her paranoid delusions were gone.

However, as Slater points out, there is no long-term follow-up on Mrs. M. (or any of Moniz’s other lobotomy patients), so it is unclear if the improvement was lasting or if there were any other side effects. According to the limited records that exist on Moniz’s first lobotomy patients, the procedure was a success: “Of the twenty original cases, Moniz claimed a complete cure for seven of them, a partial cure for another seven, and six who were unhelped” (232). The lobotomy was first introduced to American physicians in 1937, when Moniz published his findings on the lobotomy in the American Journal of Psychiatry. Two U.S.-based surgeons by the name of Walter Freeman and James Watts put their own spin on the procedure, which differed mainly in how they accessed the brain: While Moniz went through an incision at the hairline, Freeman and Watts went through the eye socket. This method was called a “transorbital lobotomy” (232). As in Portugal, the lobotomy was a reported success with patients in the U.S.

By 1949, Moniz had been awarded a Nobel Prize for the discovery of this procedure and, in the U.S. alone, it is estimated that 20,000 lobotomies were performed each year. Given its success, Slater wonders, then, why does the lobotomy have such a dark, sordid reputation. The main criticism is that it is barbaric, irreversible, and that it removes an indefinable human “vital spark.”

“Part Two” Summary

In “Part Two,” Slater examines the modern-day lobotomy:

A combination of lack of consensus in the field and its history of controversial characters has kept modern-day lobotomies, dressed up in different names, a last-ditch option for only the sickest among us, a procedure shrouded in secrecy and shame (239).

In the U.S., psychosurgery is completely outlawed in California and Oregon, and in many other states it is very difficult to get the medical approval needed in order to have a lobotomy performed. Slater, nonetheless, manages to find a man who is approved for the procedure: Charlie Newitz, a 40-year-old man in Austin, Texas, who suffers from lifelong “incapacitating” obsessive-compulsive disorder. Charlie has had numerous rounds of shock treatment therapy and tried 23 different kinds of psychiatric medications.

Slater joins Charlie on the day of his procedure in December of 1999. When Charlie’s surgery is complete, he immediately finds that his obsessive-compulsive disorder is completely gone. A week later, when Slater follows up, Charlie reports that the obsessive-compulsive disorder is still gone, but he is feeling “a little low” (247). When Slater asks if he was happy that he went through with the surgery, he responds with a resounding yes, and reports that he would do it again in a second. In fact, he wants to undergo another one, to see if that will completely alleviate his depressive symptoms. Slater concludes the chapter by noting that the controversy surrounding the lobotomy points to the fact that, for humanity, the brain is sacred:

No matter what the facts show, no matter how persistently the information points to the possible efficacy of psychosurgery and the inefficacy of medication, there is still something holy about that three-pound wrinkled walnut with a sheen (247).

Conclusion Summary

In the Conclusion, Slater writes that the mythology surrounding Deborah Skinner and her reported suicide was what inspired her to write Opening Skinner’s Box. Over the course of the book, however, Slater’s questions about psychology only deepen: “I wanted to, when I came to the end of this book, to offer up and answer, a conclusion, but as oftentimes happens in experiments, which this book ultimately is, the data yields only new domains for further exploration” (248).

Slater sees philosophical questions as the common thread among all the experiments she studied. Psychology deals with metaphysical issues:

Over and over again arise issues of free will (Skinner, Alexander, Loftus, Moniz), conformity/obedience (Milgram, Darley and Latané, Festinger, Rosenhan), and the ethics of experimentation itself on living beings (Harlow, Skinner, Milgram, Moniz). Even the most technically proficient experiments, like Kandel’s, ultimately concern themselves not with the value-free questions we traditionally associate with ‘science,’ of which psychology insists it is a part, but with the kinds of ethical and existential questions we associate with philosophy (249).

Many scientists have hesitated to classify psychology as a “true” science, due to the fact that many of the philosophical issues psychology aims to explore defy the usual categories of measurement required for scientific inquiry. Still, Slater sees usefulness in experimental psychology, despite the criticisms. She believes that, even as science advances and evolves, there will always be the desire to pursue the kind of philosophical questions explored in psychology:

But of course, there will always be new questions, if only the question about having no questions and what that means, and here we are, back to philosophy again. It seems we can’t escape. No matter how technologically proficient our newest experiments, we cannot escape the residue of mystery and muck, so we carry the residue with us (254).

Chapter 8-Conclusion Analysis

In the final section of Opening Skinner’s Box, Slater focuses on neurologically-oriented sub-fields within psychology. She explores the work of Elizabeth Loftus, Eric Kandel, and António Egas Moniz, all of whom conducted research to pinpoint the biological basis of mental functions (i.e., memory and mental illness).

As in earlier chapters, Slater draws direct connections between the psychologists’ biographical information and their respective research pursuits. In Chapter 8, Slater connects Elizabeth Loftus’s research on memory with the trauma she experienced as a child, when her mother committed suicide: “There is something split off in Loftus, unresolved, damped down, working its way out sideways. She is the survivor who questions the validity of survivorship. That’s one way out of a bind” (201). Chapter 8 dives deeply into Elizabeth Loftus’s personal history and the chapter ends with a personal letter Loftus wrote to her deceased mother. While Slater presents background information on each of the psychologists, Chapter 8 relies most heavily on Loftus’s personal history to make a larger point about the philosophical questions pursued in her work on memory.

In Chapter 9, Slater speculates that Eric Kandel’s survival of the Holocaust may have driven his desire to study memory: “One has to wonder: What role does the Holocaust play in Kandel’s lifelong dedication to the cellular study of memory?” (210). In Chapter 10, Slater writes that António Egas Moniz’s “daring” was what gave him the courage to experiment on surgically modifying the human brain. Slater’s exploration of the psychologist’s biographical data—telling the stories of their lives, in essence—is another way in which Opening Skinner’s Box uses narrative to deepen our understanding of psychology.

Psychologists have always sought to quantify their work and pinpoint a biological basis for many of their studies to align with traditional science. In Chapter 8, Slater underscores what a landmark discovery it was that the hippocampus was related to memory formation. Slater writes: “Memory was flesh. It could be pinpointed, like a country on a map. There. There lives your past. There lives your future. In the seahorse. Beneath the cortical coral reef. In one man’s silver straw” (207). In Chapters 9 and 10, the work of Eric Kandel and António Egas Moniz greatly furthered psychology’s rooting in scientific, biological basis: Kandel, through his discovery of CREB, and Moniz, with the advent of the lobotomy. The experiments Slater studies in this section are instrumental in linking psychology to the “hard” sciences.

Slater concludes with an emphasis on the philosophical angle of each psychologist’s research: In Chapter 8, Slater explores memory’s relationship to trauma. In Chapter 9, Slater ruminates on how Kandel’s research was borne from a desire to extend life indefinitely: “I think of Kandel’s little red pill and wonder if soon we will be able to undo not only aging, but death itself, a purple pill for that, would we want it?” (221). In Chapter 10, Slater asks why the lobotomy has a reputation as being “evil” (235). In the Conclusion of the book, she reinstates the idea that psychology always leads back to philosophical questions: “No matter how technologically proficient our newest experiments, we cannot escape the residue of mystery and murk, so we carry the residue with us” (254).

blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
blurred text
Unlock IconUnlock all 54 pages of this Study Guide

Plus, gain access to 8,800+ more expert-written Study Guides.

Including features:

+ Mobile App
+ Printable PDF
+ Literary AI Tools